How parents can  lessen or  prevent AUTISM symptoms in young children: Â
Before birth:Â
Ultrasounds: Â Consider limiting the number of ultrasounds you as a pregnant woman undergo. Â What is ultra sound? Â You must know that “something” is passing through highly sensitive, quickly developing human tissue. Â Do you really need to know the sex of your child? Â Does your MD really need to know exactly how when the child is likely to be born so they can plan their time better? Â Less is best for your child if merely done to know sex and date of birth.
Immunizations:  know what your MD or hospital will do right after birth concerning possible immunizations.  Does your child really need them? Learn and you  decide.  If  OK with your MD, stretch out the time between immunizations. For example instead of 3 on one day why not one shot  a week over the course of a month.
Improve & maintain your health while pregnant and after birth: what possible problems are likely given your family history? Depression? High blood pressure? Diabetes? Get prenatal medical advise. Know the  kinds of vitamins and what you can do to be optimally healthy.
After birth:Â
Establish rhythms between child and parents and other care takers.  So that the child knows by the actions that occur  that someone will pick the child up, sooth with  their well known voices heard inutero and  feed and change the child as needed.
Talk to your child. Do so before birth and do so after birth. Keep it simple. Speak clearly and slowly. Look the child in their eyes when talking to them. From birth on, speak to the child with simple, caring language:  “Are you hungry?”  “Want to play?” “Time to stretch” ? “Are you wet?”  “How about a hug and kiss?” “Listen to this song.”  Use the words from birth that you want them to learn.  Keep it simple.  Lots of reinforcement.
Tell the child what you are doing and give the child time to respond to you.  Say, “Time for milk …”  or “Time to eat …”  and look at the child so the child can anticipate a bottle or breast. Connect your words and nonverbal actions so they child comes to expect what will occur having heard  your words and seen your actions. Anticipation is healthy for your child.
Know & respect the very different  temperments  young children have:  Is your child slow to warm up?  Highly active physically? Very regular in what they want for sleep and food?  Or is your child highly irregular and changeable moment to moment. You as the parent have to be flexible and — even though tired — willing to adapt to the needs of your child.
Create a warm, colorful, inviting physical and emotional envirnment for your baby:Â Â things that dangle. Â colors and shapes. soft and hard objects to touch. music that sooths in the background. darkness for sleep and light and sound for day time contact.
Get help:
Plan ahead and be nice to your mother-in-law or other persons who could step in and assist in the first few weeks. Â Plan ahead so mother and father can have a break. Â And, assuming your in-laws have had children (how else could you be there) also assume that they can act appropriately with your young bundle of love. Let them use their wisdom and knowledge and don’t make them do what you do. Â Learn from your elders. Yes, what a thought for young mothers and fathers following the latest fads in child raising.Â
What to know: The variations in normal development are immense.
Information about  the development of your child is vital:  Do keep track of your child’s development with smart phone cameras and old fashioned baby books.  Write down: age child first smiles? age child starts to kick arms and legs to get your attention? age child turns head towards you when you say his or her name?
Well Baby Checks:Write out a list of any concerns you have and take that list when you talk to you MD. Â There are excellent check lists MDs can have you fill out concerning the issues that may suggest delays in social and communication development.
Seek early help:  There are twenty plus Regional Centers in California which  provide assistance for  young children  with developmental delays.  Parents can self refer or they can have their MD refer the child.  No diagnosis is necessary to get excellent assistance to assist your child’s development in the 0 to 3 age period.
This is just to get you going. Â There are lots of books out there. If you have questions or comments sent them to: Â Â DrCameronJackson@gmail.comÂ
Dr. Jackson is a licensed California psychologist. She is employed part time for one of California’s regional centers. Dr. Jackson assesses children and adults and provides short-term therapy. Dr. Jackson has expertise in autism and other developmental delays. You can find additional information about Dr. Jackson on Monterey Bay Forum www.FreedomOK.net and on the Psychology Today website. Â She may be contacted at P.O.Box 1972, Aptos, CA 95003 Â 831 688-6002
How to potty train children with autism and other developmental disabilities go to http://www.avakid.com
“This unique potty training app provides a simple menu to create a cartoon avatar that physically resembles your child/children, and then enables your child to repeatedly play the Go Potty narrative showing him/herself successfully complete the whole process of using the potty step by step. There is also an accident scene. The layout is intentionally simple and free of distractions so your child’s attention will be focused on him/herself using the potty. The app includes 15 actionable potty training tips including concrete advice about preparation, behavioral reinforcement, behavioral shaping, when to continue vs. take a break, and more.”
Information on this potty app was found on Autism Speaks:
Good news for CA children with symptoms of autism.   Because of SB 946, CA parents  who persist may  get treatment paid by their health insurance. But parents may have to jump through several hoops!
Here’s how: 1)   Ask your health care provider for interventions to reduce symptoms of autism.   2) If you are told that services are not necessary,  ask that your child be assessed.  3) If denied an  assessment, you may appeal the denial. 4) If your health plan denies appeal  –or does not respond in 30 days  –you may request an independent medical review (IMR) from the Department of Managed Health Care in California.
So are you willing to persist? Below are links to help:  Â
All that glitters is not gold.The ADOS has been likened to the ‘gold standard’ for assessment of autism. Â Not so. Â The ADOS is more akin to ‘fool’s gold’ than real gold.
The ADOS used to assess for autism in young children is frequently misused by many professionals. Watch out, parents!
It’s time to weigh carefully the usefulness of the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, known as the ADOS, an instrument commonly used to diagnosis autism.
In advertisements, the ADOS is referred to as ‘the gold standard’ for diagnosis of autism. See the ad at Western Psychological Service:
But is it the real McCoy? Perhaps, if used correctly. But far too often the ADOS is misused.
An apt description of the ADOS is that it’s akin to ‘fool’s gold’. And little weight should be placed on the results of an ADOS unless administered as the author Dr. Lord intended – as frosting on a cake already baked, i.e., as something that supplements other test instrument already given.
So, if a Wechsler IQ has already been done in conjunction with a speech instrument such as the PLS-4 then, yes, an ADOS can assist in providing useful information how this person communicates and socially interacts.
The ADOS was designed as a research tool used in a university setting with the collaboration of various specialists.
The ADOS was not intended to be used as a stand-alone instrument. But, too frequently it is the only test administered. And that’s when the ‘gold’ the ADOS might provide turns into ‘fools gold’.
To illustrate the above, here’s a true story. Some facts and dates have been changed to protect the family’s privacy.
Joe (not his true name) was placed in pre-school in Santa Cruz County, California. After a few weeks his parents were told that the school could not provide what he needed and were asked to withdraw him. That brief pre-school experience is the only pr e-school experience Joe had.
Joe started Kindergarten  and his school assessed him for special education assistance. The assessment by the school included two tests of IQ, a test of memory, the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS), the Gilliam Autism Rating Scale (GARS) and the Gilliam Asperger’s Disorder Scale (GADS). Based on that psycho-educational assessment, his school made Joe eligible for special education services under the category ‘autistic-like’ . But “autistic-like ‘ is not a diagnosis. It is an education category.
To obtain more assistance Joe’s parents used private insurance to get further assessment. Concerned with Joe’s slow language development, Joe’s mother took him to their pediatrician who referred Joe in-house to a developmental pediatrician.
Joe was seen by a Palo Alto Medical Foundation Developmental Behavioral Pediatrician. The family traveled to the Bay Area for the assessment.
The Developmental Behavioral Pediatrician did not ask for — and therefore did not review — the psycho-educational assessment done a year before by Joe’s school. Thus the rich ‘cake’ of information which the ADOS could have supplemented as ‘frosting’ was not part of the assessment process.
A Palo Alto Medical Foundation www.pamf.org/  specialist MD provided Joe’s parents with a 3 page report, most of it a template in which two paragraphs pertinent to Joe were inserted. The report stated that an ADOS Module 2 was administered. None of the 12 measured ADOS activities were referenced and no summary numbers were provided to show how Joe performed. The MD stated that Joe’s performance on the ADOS supports a diagnosis of autistic spectrum disorder. A paragraph later the MD then then stated that Joe met criteria for DSM-IV diagnosis of Autistic Disorder. Yet no specific information was provided to show that the criteria for a diagnosis of Autistic Disorder was met.
This is an example how the ADOS is used as a stand-alone instrument for the diagnosis of autism. Dr. Lord did not intend that the ADOS be used in this manner. But it often is and therefore it is misused.Â
Joe’s parents will probably use this diagnosis – based strictly on a stand-alone ADOS with little supportive facts — to seek additional services for Joe under the new laws in California that require insurance companies to provide treatment for autism.
Now you know one reason why there has been an explosion of autism: the mis-use of the ADOS as a test used in diagnosing autism.
And to end on a positive note:
All that is gold does not glitter,
Not all those who wander are lost;
The old that is strong does not wither,
Deep roots are not reached by the frost.
The best assessment for autism — for cancer and for any disorder — is based on multiple sources of information done to show development over time and uses both standardized tests as well as other appropriate methods.
Sub-standard assessments for autism — such as the above example — are done using one stand-alone test not meant to be so used with no review of prior records and no use of available standardized instruments.
The Palo Alto Medical Foundation www.pamf.org/  is one of several large medical institutions in California that on occasion provide sloppy assessments to the public — ones which don’t meet Best Practices standards. And when that happens their toes should be held to the fire.
You can always complain to the appropriate oversight organizations. Go first to the MD and ask questions.  T hen contact available standards organizations.   If that does not work go to state agencies.
If you have a legitimate, substantial complaint concerning medical care contact  www.mbc.ca.gov/Â
Dr. Lord will be publishing a new ADOS for toddlers expected to be available in the Fall of 2012.
So more to come about how well the ADOS measures autism in toddlers and young children.
written by Cameron Jackson, Ph.D. DrCameronJackson@gmail.com
More below on the ADOS for toddler written in 2009 by Nestor Lopez-Duran. Search under ADOS for his article.
Diagnosing autism in toddlers: The new ADOS Toddler Module enters the field
By Nestor Lopez-Duran PhD On May 20, 2009 · 7 Comments
The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (The ADOS) is a diagnostic instrument that was created by the University of Michigan Professor Dr. Cathy Lord. During the last 2 decades, the ADOS has become the most accepted diagnostic tool for autism spectrum disorders. The ADOS has 4 different overlapping ‘versions’ (or modules) that were designed to be used with individuals of various ages and abilities – including non-verbal individuals. However, the original ADOS is not very useful in the diagnosis of children under the age of 3. For these children, the ADOS is not specific enough. That is, it incorrectly identifies ASD in many children who actually have a non-ASD developmental delays.
But why do we need an autism diagnostic instrument for children under 3?
There are actually a number of valid and important reasons in support of the early diagnosis of autism. When conducting evaluations of children with autism I hear parents describe how they knew that ‘something was wrong’ since their child was very young. This phenomena is not just a clinical anecdote, as it has supported by research studies (see for example Chawarska et al. 2007 DOI:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.0185.x) suggesting that in some children, clear symptoms of autism can be identified very early. In addition, a number of studies have shown that early intervention is extremely important in the treatment of autism, thus early identification would help families obtain the interventions they need.
Given the need to have a diagnostic instrument that can be used with children under 3, Dr. Lord and her team at the University of Michigan have been working on a new ADOS module that would reliably identify autism in these young children. The results of these efforts have now been presented in an article to be published in the Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. In the article, the authors described in detail the process that led to the development of the ADOS new toddler module (ADOS-Module T). However, I will limit this post to a description of the validation procedures.
In order to test this new module, the authors used the ADOS-Module T in 360 clinical evaluations with children under age 3 conducted at the University of Michigan Autism and Communication Disorders Clinic, and at the University of California-San Diego Autism Center of Excellence. These children included those who eventually would receive a diagnosis of non-ASD developmental delays, ASDs, or no diagnosis at all (typically developing). The ASD children had their clinical diagnoses of ASD based on a “best estimate†procedure conducted by specialists, and based partly on a modified version of the ADI-R. The non-ASD developmental disorder group as well as the typically developing group were also evaluated for ASDs with the ADI and they did not meet standards for ASDs.
So in essence, the ADOS-Module T was employed on 3 groups of children: Children with ASD, children with a non-ASD disorder, and typically developing children. The clinicians administering and scoring the ADOS-Module T were unaware of the eventual diagnoses of these children. This allows the researchers to examine the specificity and sensitivity of the new ADOS module in the correct identification of autism spectrum disorders.
What is sensitivity and specificity? Sensitivity refers to how accurate the instrument is in the identification of autism when autism exist. For example, when a test has 80% sensitivity, this means that 80% of the time when a condition is present the test will ‘catch it’. Specificity however, refers to how well the test differentiates the target condition from other conditions. So for example, a test may have very high sensitivity in that every time the target condition (in this case autism) is present, the test gives you a ‘positive’ result. But the same test my have very low specificity, in that it also gives you a positive result when a different condition is present, so that it incorrectly identifies the target condition as present when it’s not there!
How did the new ADOS Module T perform?
The sensitivity of the ADOS Toddler module was 91%. That is, the test was able to correctly identify 91% of the cases of ASD (based on a cut off score of 12).
The specificity of the ADOS-Module T when tested against non-ASD disorders was also 91%. This means that only in 9% of the cases, the test suggested a diagnosis when the child had been previously identified as not having an ASD.
The specificity of the ADOS-Module T when tested against typically developing cases was 94%. That is, only in 6% of the cases, the test suggested a diagnosis in children who were actually typically developing kids.
These are actually excellent numbers and indicate that the ADOS Toddler Module has excellent sensitivity and specificity. However, the authors also described some general concerns and limitations.
The ADOS, although it is the most reliable and valid diagnostic instrument available, it is still only a clinical tool that must be used in the context of a comprehensive clinical evaluation and it is subservient to clinical judgment. Specifically, a diagnosis of autism is provided only when the person meets the DSM-IV diagnostic criteria. Therefore, clinicians must use their judgments in interpreting and applying the results from the ADOS. There will be cases when the ADOS suggests a diagnosis but the clinician will not provide the diagnosis because the child doesn’t meet full diagnostic criteria based on the DSM-IV.
So you may ask, what is the point? Why do we have the ADOS if all a clinician has to do is go down the list of the DSM-IV criteria and add up the check marks? The ADOS provides for a reliable and valid tool to assess for the specific symptoms included in the DSM-IV criteria and it helps the clinician interpret the child’s clinical presentation as it applies to the DSM-IV criteria. The ADOS standardizes this process so that diagnoses are less dependent upon other factors, such as biases in parental reporting of symptoms, or the skills or training of the clinician in properly indentifying such symptoms. Therefore, the ADOS greatly improves the validity and accuracy of our ‘clinical judgment’.
On a personal note and disclaimer. Dr. Luyster (lead author of the study), Dr. Richler, and Dr. Oti were all my classmates in graduate school and I congratulate them for their wonderful work. In addition Dr. Lord, creator of the ADOS and founder of the University of Michigan Autism and Communication Disorders Clinic, will be my collague this Fall when I join the University of Michigan faculty.
The Reference: Luyster, R., Gotham, K., Guthrie, W., Coffing, M., Petrak, R., Pierce, K., Bishop, S., Esler, A., Hus, V., Oti, R., Richler, J., Risi, S., & Lord, C. (2009). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule—Toddler Module: A New Module of a Standardized Diagnostic Measure for Autism Spectrum Disorders Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders DOI: 10.1007/s10803-009-0746-zResearchBlogging.org
Post to Twitter
Tagged with: Autism • Toddlers
If you enjoyed this article, please consider sharing it!
Reddit Facebook Twitter Delicious StumbleUpon
7 Responses to Diagnosing autism in toddlers: The new ADOS Toddler Module enters the field
Brandon says:
May 20, 2009 at 1:43 pm
Very interesting. Mr. Lopez I hope you enjoy coming to the U of M. I am in the Adult Autism Social Group up there, so I know some of the researchers. I find the ADOS-T very interesting I will be sure to ask Katie Gotham more about it.
Reply
Nestor Lopez-Duran PhD says:
May 20, 2009 at 4:11 pm
Thank you Brandon for your comment. I’m sure I will enjoy Michigan. I went to graduate school there and coming back to join the faculty feels like coming back home. 🙂 Cheers, Nestor.
Reply
JulieL says:
May 22, 2009 at 9:29 pm
I listed to a recent podcast with Professor Margot Prior. She noted that there is recent research in the US and UK stating that children can be, as she stated “picked up as at risk†for autism, at the age 12-24 months old. She was clear to state that this was not to say these would be always accurate, but it would be say a marker for close observation for that child. Do you know of anything regarding the research she speaks of? This new research article you present here seems to be adding to that body of agreement.
Thanks 🙂
Reply
Pregnancy Questions says:
June 16, 2009 at 11:45 pm
Do you plan to keep this site updated? I sure hope so…it’s great.
Reply
Naomi Weatherford says:
August 15, 2011 at 1:30 pm
Very interesting article Dr. Lopez-Duran.
I am caring for a child who is believed to have been neglected for 15 months and at 30 months continues to have significant delays in speech, coordination, and social development. Is there a reliable tool you are aware of to determine if the child has an autism spectrum disorder versus continued delay due to maltreatment?
Reply
Nestor Lopez-Duran PhD says:
August 17, 2011 at 3:14 pm
Hi Naomi, at 30-months a local pediatric neuropsychologist or autism specialist could provide an evaluation that could guide you in obtaining the necessary services. Most professionals may be a bit reluctant to provide a diagnosis at that age, but they can identify the pattern of strengths and weakness that could help him/her receive the best intervention at this time. Thank you. Nestor.
AND EVEN MORE ON THE TODDLER ADOS:
J Autism Dev Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 September 1.
Published in final edited form as:
J Autism Dev Disord. 2009 September; 39(9): 1305–1320.
Published online 2009 May 5. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0746-z
PMCID: PMC2893552
NIHMSID: NIHMS211126
The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule – Toddler Module: A new module of a standardized diagnostic measure for autism spectrum disorders
Rhiannon Luyster, Katherine Gotham, Whitney Guthrie, Mia Coffing, Rachel Petrak, Karen Pierce, Somer Bishop, Amy Esler, Vanessa Hus, Rosalind Oti, Jennifer Richler, Susan Risi, and Catherine Lord
Author information â–º Copyright and License information â–º
The publisher’s final edited version of this article is available at J Autism Dev Disord
See other articles in PMC that cite the published article.
Go to:
Abstract
The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS; Lord et al., 2000) is widely accepted as a “gold standard†diagnostic instrument, but it is of restricted utility with very young children. The purpose of the current project was to modify the ADOS for use in children under 30 months of age. A modified ADOS, the ADOS Toddler Module (or Module T), was used in 360 evaluations. Participants included 182 children with best estimate diagnoses of ASD, non-spectrum developmental delay or typical development. A final set of protocol and algorithm items was selected based on their ability to discriminate the diagnostic groups. The traditional algorithm “cutoffs†approach yielded high sensitivity and specificity, and a new range of concern approach was proposed.
Keywords: autism spectrum disorders, diagnosis, ADOS, infants, toddlers
Almost ten years ago, the standardization of a revised Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS), a semi-structured assessment for the diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders (ASD) (Lord, Rutter, DiLavore & Risi, 1999) was described. The ADOS has gradually become an integral part of many research and clinical protocols of children suspected of having an autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Due to the growing understanding of symptoms in the first two years of life and the desire of researchers and clinicians to have standardized instruments for use with infants and young toddlers, there is a need for diagnostic tools that are appropriate for very young children.
This paper presents a new Toddler Module of the ADOS. The Toddler Module retains the original spirit and many of the original tasks of the ADOS, but is intended for use in children under 30 months of age who have nonverbal mental ages of at least 12 months. The scope of this report is to provide a summary of the new measure, the procedures used to develop it, a description of the standardization sample and relevant psychometrics.
In introducing this new module, it is valuable to review the structure of the previously published ADOS. The ADOS evaluates social interaction, communication and play through a series of planned “presses†(Lord et al., 1989) in the context of a naturalistic social interaction. Some of the presses are intended to offer a high level of structure for the participant, while others are intended to provide less structure. All presses, however, afford contexts for both initiations and responses, which are then coded in a standardized manner. An algorithm, which sums the scores of particular items from the measure, yields a classification indicative of autism, ASD or non-spectrum conditions. This classification can then be used by a clinician or researcher as one part of a comprehensive diagnostic process.
The first ADOS was introduced in the late 1980s and was intended for children who had a spoken language age equivalent of at least 36 months. A revision was published in 2000 that reflected the need for the measure to be applicable to a wider range of chronological and developmental ages. The 2000 version provided four separate (but overlapping) modules for individuals of different ages and language abilities. The updated ADOS did indeed extend the usefulness of the original ADOS below a language age of 3 years, but research has indicated that it remains of limited value for children with nonverbal mental ages below 16 months (Gotham, Risi, Pickles & Lord, 2007). For this young population, the ADOS Module 1 algorithm is over-inclusive, meaning that it classifies about 81 percent (19% specificity) of children with intellectual disabilities and/or language impairments as having autism or ASD when clinical judgment deems that they do not. Revised Module 1 algorithms (Gotham et al., 2007; Gotham et al., 2008) improve specificity but only to 50%.
In recent years, it is precisely this age range, the first two years of life, that has become one of the central concentrations of autism research efforts. Researchers have used creative methodologies to explore the early differences in children who are later diagnosed with ASD, including retrospective videotape analysis, as well as the identification of infants at high risk for ASD (usually the younger siblings of children diagnosed with ASD). The ADOS has been of limited use in these projects, because many of the children fell chronologically or developmentally below the floor of the measure. However, a number of standardized direct observational measures have been developed for use with young children at risk for ASD, such as the Screening Tool for Autism in Two-Year-Olds (STAT; Stone, Coonrod, Turner & Pozdol, 2004), the Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales Developmental Profile (CSBS-DP; Wetherby, 2001) and the Autism Observational Scale for Infants (AOSI; Bryson, Zwaigenbaum, McDermott, Rombough & Brian, 2008).
Each of these measures serves a different purpose within ASD research. They vary in terms of their target age range, whether they are intended to be used as a screening or diagnostic measure, and whether they were designed to be ASD-specific. The STAT is one of many measures which are intended to be ASD-specific screeners, used in clinics or other specialty centers to identify children at-risk for ASD. It is not intended to be a diagnostic measure, and it is designed for use with children 24 to 35 months of age. The CSBS is intended to be a screening and evaluation measure of communication, social and symbolic abilities in a broad population of children, including children with ASD as well as those with other, non-spectrum conditions. It is designed to identify children (between 12 and 24 months of age) at risk for general developmental delay rather than ASD in particular. However, the use of a specific scoring system (the Systematic Observation of Red Flags) with the CSBS allows for the identification of children at risk for ASD (Wetherby et al., 2004). The AOSI is intended to be an ASD-specific measure, used to detect symptoms of ASD in children between 6 and 18 months of age. Although it may eventually be established as a diagnostic measure, it is not yet proposed to be used as such (Bryson, Zwaigenbaum, McDermott, Rombough, & Brian, 2008). Therefore, although these (and other) important measures for young children have been established, none of them offer a standardized way to reach a diagnostic classification for very young children suspected of having ASD.
A standardized diagnostic measure applicable for infants and young toddlers is also needed for early identification efforts. As public awareness of ASD heightens, parents have been more likely to seek out an evaluation for their very young children. The average age of parental concern is between 15 and 18 months (Chawarska, Paul et al., 2007; DeGiacomo & Fombonne, 1998), and some parents (particularly those who already have one child on the spectrum) have concerns about their child from the earliest months of life. Early identification has been strongly promoted by federal and advocacy organizations with the idea that earlier provision of services will be associated with better outcomes. These findings all point to the need for professionals to be equipped to handle diagnostic assessments for very young children. The Toddler Module should be a useful component of such assessments. One caveat, however, is that diagnostic decisions made very early in life are less stable than those made, for instance, at ages closer to 3 years (Charman et al., 2005; Turner & Stone, 2007). This has been taken into consideration in recommendations for interpretation of the Toddler Module scores (discussed below).
The Toddler Module offers new and modified ADOS activities and scores appropriate for children under 30 months of age who have minimal speech (ranging from no spoken words to simple two-word phrases), have a nonverbal age equivalent of at least 12 months and are walking independently. Communication, reciprocal social interaction, and emerging object use and/or play skills are all targeted by the module. The ADOS, particularly Modules 1 and 2 – which are intended for developmentally younger children – is designed around the general model that the examiner presents loosely structured and highly motivating materials and activities (e.g., bubbles, snack, remote activated toys) in order to see how the child responds, and whether he/she then makes initiations in order to maintain the interaction.
As in the previously published ADOS modules, each activity of the Toddler Module provides a hierarchy of presses for the examiner. Items that were judged to be appropriate for infants and toddlers were selected from Module 1 of the ADOS and from the PL-ADOS, an early version of the ADOS intended for pre-verbal children (DiLavore, Lord & Rutter, 1995). Additional activities and codes were written based on a review of empirical studies on early development (Behne, Carpenter, Call & Tomasello, 2005; Phillips, Baron-Cohen & Rutter, 1992). Some of the items from previous ADOS versions were re-written to be more appropriate for younger children, and all codes were written on a 4-point scale, ranging from ‘0’ (no evidence of abnormality related to autism) to ‘3’ (definite evidence, such that behavior interferes with interaction). Eleven activities are included in the Toddler Module (see Table 1), and there are 41 accompanying ratings.
Table 1
Table 1
Toddler Module Activities
The Toddler Module follows the same basic structure as the Module 1. It should be conducted in a small child-friendly room, and a familiar caregiver should always be present. Simpler cause-and-effect materials are included as well as toys that require the development of more representational and/or imaginative play. Because some of the Module 1 activities – such as a pretend birthday party – may be unfamiliar to younger children, everyday contexts (i.e., a bath-time routine) have been substituted.1
Another substantial design change was made because younger children may make fewer explicit and directed initiations towards an unfamiliar adult than older children (Sroufe, 1977). Consequently, in the Toddler Module, we have added instances of the examiner structuring an interaction and waiting for a minimal change in the child’s behavior, such as a shift in gaze, facial expression or vocalization. These new activities require less complex motor responses than the Module 1 tasks.
As with other ADOS modules, detailed notes should be taken by the examiner during administration, and coding should be done immediately after the module is complete. Perhaps even more so than other modules, the success and validity of the Toddler Module is dependent on the skill of the examiner. Infants and toddlers, whether typically developing or not, are particularly sensitive to the introduction of new situations and new people (Bohlin & Hagekull, 1993). Indeed, this age range is associated with the development of important components of social and environmental awareness, such as stranger anxiety. As such, the validity of the Toddler Module assumes the clinical skills required to navigate the needs of very young children and carry out the administration and scoring in a reliable fashion.
Design Decisions
Pilot analyses indicated that children chronologically and developmentally younger than 12 months of age consistently obtained elevated scores on early versions of the Toddler Module items, regardless of best estimate diagnostic group. We therefore set a lower cutoff of 12 months nonverbal mental age. In order to allow for the possibility of including children who were performing at age-level, we similarly set the lower cutoff of 12 months chronological age. However, it was anticipated that the final sample would include very few children in the ASD group who approached this lower chronological age cutoff, due to the commonality of developmental delays in children with ASD.
It was also necessary to determine at what developmental point children should receive the Module 1, rather than the Toddler Module. Preliminary analyses indicated that Module 1 ADOS sensitivity (percent of children with ASD exceeding the cutoff) and specificity (percent of children without ASD falling below the cutoff) for children over the age of 30 months was superior to the Toddler Module. For this reason, children over 30 months of age were not included in any further analyses, and the methods and results described below exclude these older children. Once a child is over the age of 30 months, he/she should receive the Module 1 of the ADOS (assuming that the child does not yet have sufficient language for a Module 2). Upon mastering spontaneous, non-echoed phrases made up of three independent units, regardless of age, a child should receive the Module 2 of the ADOS.
Diagnostic Algorithm
A subset of items comprise the diagnostic algorithms (see Table 2), following the format of the other modules. Algorithm items are structured according to the domains used in the revised ADOS algorithms (Gotham et al., 2007): Social Affect and Restricted, Repetitive Behaviors. These two domains have been shown to better capture the factor structure of ADOS data than the original three-factor structure (Gotham et al., 2008; Gotham, Risi, Pickles, & Lord, 2007). All items contribute to one overall score with a single diagnostic cutoff.
Table 2
Table 2
Algorithm Items
Recent research has indicated that early diagnostic classification within the autism spectrum (making a distinction between the specific diagnoses of autism and pervasive developmental disorder – not otherwise specified, or PDD-NOS) is relatively unstable in young children, even though diagnoses of ASD more broadly versus other, non-spectrum disorders are consistent over time. Lord et al. (2006) reported that 14 percent of children diagnosed with autism at age 2 shifted their diagnosis to PDD-NOS by age 9. Moreover, in children with an age 2 diagnosis of PDD-NOS, 60 percent shifted into an autism classification by age 9. Turner and colleagues (2006), using another sample, reported similar levels of diagnostic uncertainty within the autism spectrum but in the opposite direction, as have other more recent investigations (Kleinman et al., 2008).
Consequently, the Toddler Module includes only two classifications intended for research use: ASD or non-spectrum. Because of the newness of these methods, the relatively small sample sizes, and the care required in interpreting these results, the emphasis for clinical interpretation is on ranges of scores associated with each algorithm. These ranges are associated with the need for clinical monitoring and follow-up (rather than a focus on a cutoff for ASD) and can reflect little-or-no, mild-to-moderate, or moderate-to-severe concern.
The purpose of the ADOS algorithm is to provide a classification for the child’s current ASD diagnostic status. In the long run, the predictive validity of these scores is extremely important but beyond the scope of this paper and will need additional follow-up data from this and other projects. As with the rest of the ADOS, the algorithm score should never be used as the only source of information in generating a diagnosis. Details about a child’s developmental history, parent descriptions and current cognitive, social, language and adaptive functioning across a variety of contexts, as well as the judgment of a skilled clinician, are all necessary for appropriate diagnosis and recommendations (National Research Council, 2001).
Go to:
Method
Participants
The sample included all children between the ages of 12 and 30 months from three sources: (1) consecutive referrals of children from 12 to 30 months of age from the clinic at the University of Michigan Autism and Communication Disorders Center, (2) children from University of Michigan projects studying early development of children with communication delays and/or at risk for ASD (predominantly younger siblings of children on the autism spectrum), as well as comparison groups of children recruited for these projects and (3) children participating in research at the University of California – San Diego Autism Center of Excellence. “Best estimate†clinical or research diagnoses were assigned based on clinical impressions of a clinical psychologist or an advanced graduate student in psychology (who had received at least two years of supervised ASD-specific assessment and diagnostic experience). Information from a research version of the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R, a parent interview; Rutter, Le Couteur & Lord, 2003), modified to be appropriate for toddlers (see Lord, Shulman & DiLavore, 2004) and direct observation (which included the Toddler Module and standardized language and cognitive testing) was available. Thus, clinical diagnosis was not independent of the ADOS. However, algorithms were not derived until after the samples were collected.
The final sample included data from 162 participants at the University of Michigan Autism and Communication Disorders Center; and data from an additional 20 participants from the University of California, San Diego. Preliminary analyses indicated no site differences in age, developmental level or algorithm scores, both within and across diagnostic groups. The project included children with typical development (TD), non-spectrum disorders (NS) and ASD. All individuals with NS and TD did not meet standard ADI-R criteria for ASD (Risi et al., 2006) and received best estimate diagnoses outside the autism spectrum. Non-spectrum participants had a range of diagnoses, including 14 children with expressive language disorders, 5 children with mixed receptive-expressive language disorders, 9 children with non-specific intellectual disability, 4 children with Down syndrome, and 1 child with Fetal Alcohol Syndrome. In addition, one child had been diagnosed with chromosomal abnormalities, one with anxiety disorder not otherwise specified, one with communication disorder – not otherwise specified, and one with phonological disorder. These children were included to demonstrate that the Toddler Module does not consistently identify ASD in children with similar developmental levels as the ASD sample but who did not have ASD. Included in the sample were thirty-five younger siblings of children with ASD, 19 of whom had themselves been given a diagnosis of ASD, 11 of whom were identified as typically developing and 5 of whom had been diagnosed as non-spectrum.
As part of ongoing longitudinal studies, many participants from each site were seen more than once. These children were seen by a familiar clinician for most of their monthly visits but were evaluated by new clinician every six months, who was blind to their previous performance and tentative diagnosis. Altogether, data were used for 182 individuals, who were seen 360 times in total. There was an average of 2.01 (SD = 2.48, range = 1 to 14) assessments per participant. Children in the ASD group were seen between 1 and 14 times (M = 3.24, SD = 3.48), children in the NS group were seen between 1 and 12 times (M = 2.43, SD = 2.86), and typically developing children were seen between 1 and 12 times (M = 1.29, SD = 1.26). For the majority of the validity and reliability analyses reported below, data were analyzed separately for two groups defined by verbal status during the assessment (“verbal†included children whose scores on the item “Overall Level of Language†were ‘0’=Regular use utterances of two or more words; ‘1’=Occasional phrases, mostly single words; or ‘2’=At least five single words or word approximations; “nonverbal†included children whose scores on this item were either ‘3’= Less than five but at least one word or word approximation or ‘8’=No spontaneous words or word approximations).
Score distributions differed according to verbal/nonverbal status in children between 21 and 30 months of age. However, distributions of scores for participants younger than 21 months did not systematically vary by verbal/nonverbal status and generally resembled those of nonverbal participants aged 21–30 months. Therefore, the developmental groups were assigned as follows: (1) all children between 12 and 20 months of age as well as nonverbal children between 21 and 30 months of age (hereafter referred to as “12–20/NV21–30â€); and (2) verbal children between 21 and 30 months of age (“V21–30â€). Data were only used for one time point for each child (the assessment which included cognitive evaluations was selected for inclusion), so that participants were only represented once in each developmental group. However, the same participant could be included once in both groups (i.e., 12–20/NV21–30 and V21–30). There were 136 participants in the 12–20/NV21–30 group (113 children between 12 and 20 months and 23 nonverbal children between 21 and 30 months) and 71 participants in the V21–30 group. This set of data in which each participant was represented only once per group was termed “Unique Participants.†In the “Unique Participants†groups, the average chronological age and/or nonverbal mental age were approximately equivalent across the three diagnostic groups. As anticipated, however, there were fewer very young (i.e., under 15 months of age) children in the ASD (n=1) and NS (n=9) groups than in the TD (n=26) group. See Table 3 for sample characteristics.
Table 3
Table 3
Description of “Unique Participants†sample in validity analyses
Analyses were also run for data from all assessments for all participants in order to take advantage of the larger sample size afforded by including repeated measurements. For these analyses, there were 240 visits in 12–20/NV21–30 (194 visits from children between 12 and 20 months and 46 visits from nonverbal children between 21 and 30 months), and 122 visits in V21–30(see Table 4). This set of groups was termed “All Visits.†For these analyses, groups were generally not equivalent on measures of mental age and may have been affected by recruitment biases (e.g., non-spectrum children with more ASD-like symptoms were seen more frequently than children with non-spectrum diagnoses and fewer ASD-related behaviors).
Table 4
Table 4
Description of “All Cases†sample in validity analyses
For children who had more than one assessment in the last six months of the project, all available data, including research diagnosis history over the most recent months and chart notes, were used by two examiners to generate consensus best estimate “working diagnoses.†More weight was given to most recent diagnosis and “blind diagnoses†made by an examiner not familiar with the child. The average age of diagnosis in the 12–20/NV21–30 sample was 16.27 months (SD=2.71) in the typically developing group, 20.26 months (SD=6.12) in the non-spectrum group and 24.68 (SD=5.44) in the ASD group. In the V21–30 sample, the average age of diagnosis was 22.33 months (SD=2.67) for the typically developing group, 25.00 months (SD=5.35) in the non-spectrum group and 25.33 months (SD=2.90) in the ASD group. Each participant received a minimum of one psychometric evaluation using the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (Mullen, 1995), which yielded verbal and nonverbal language age equivalents. For children with repeated assessments, the Mullen was re-administered every six months. All participants were ambulatory (preliminary results indicated that children who were not yet walking had inflated scores on ADOS items), and none had sensory (visual or hearing) impairments or severe motor impairments.
Procedures
The Toddler Module was administered as part of an assessment by clinical research staff and was scored immediately after administration was complete. Over the course of 43 months, 18 different examiners participated in this study. These examiners all had worked with young children on the autism spectrum intensively in either research or clinical settings for at least two years. Included in this group were advanced graduate students who had both extensively observed and been directly supervised in ASD assessment and diagnosis. All examiners observed and coded numerous Toddler Modules and had attained three consecutive scorings of at least 80% exact agreement with other reliable coders on item-level scores (at least two of which had to be their own administrations) prior to becoming an independent examiner.
Testing was generally administered in a research room, with tables and chairs appropriate for young children. A familiar caregiver was always present in the room. Coding of the Toddler Module was based solely on the behaviors that occurred during the administration of the measure. This included observations of whether a child was “verbal†(i.e., used phrases or at least 5 single words or word approximations). Behaviors that occurred outside the assessment or during administration of another measure were not considered. Consent, which was approved by the University of Michigan Medical School Institutional Review Board for Human Subject Research or the University of California – San Diego Human Subjects Research Protection Program, was given by parents. Families in longitudinal projects received oral feedback and a brief report; participants in other studies received a gift card to a local store.
Inter-rater reliability for the final version of the Toddler Module was formally assessed using 14 administrations from 13 children (one child contributed two administrations). The administrations were independently coded from videotape by each of seven independent, “blind†raters from the original group of 18 examiners. The videos were selected on the basis of the quality of the recording and because the children were not known to the reliability coders. Eight of these participants had best estimate diagnoses of ASD, 3 participants were typically developing, 1 had a diagnosis of mental retardation, and 1 had a diagnosis of Down syndrome.
Go to:
Results
Test construction and pilot testing
Numerous drafts of the Toddler Module were generated and evaluated, yielding preliminary results and allowing structural decisions about the measure. Proposed items (some of which are included in the final versions and some of which have been eliminated) were used during child assessments and were reviewed and revised during weekly meetings of clinical and research staff. As the project progressed, new codes were added in order to capture additional aspects of child behavior. New examiners and examiners who previously established reliability on ADOS Modules 1 and 2 then established 80 percent agreement in pairs of raters on each item in order to ensure that inter-rater reliability could be obtained by new administrators.
Distributions of scores on each item were generated within cells of children grouped by chronological age, verbal level and diagnosis. Items which appeared to be “too hard†or “too easy†– that is, where typically developing children were often scoring in the ‘2’ to ‘3’ range or where children with ASD were frequently scoring in the ‘0’ to ‘1’ range – were re-written. Additionally, items where the scores fell only between ‘0’ and ‘2’ (that is, few children were scoring in the ‘3’ category) were revised to expand the distribution. Items were eliminated if their distributions, even after revision, did not successfully distinguish among the diagnostic groups (ASD versus typically developing and ASD versus non-spectrum) using one-way ANOVAs. The few exceptions to this criterion were items which were low-incidence but deemed to be clinically significant (e.g., self injurious behavior). When all item revisions were complete, two researchers (blind to child diagnosis) reviewed all relevant the videotaped administrations and/or notes to re-score the revised items according to the final item structure.
In order to determine if there were clinician-related effects on diagnostic decisions, a binary logistic regression was conducted predicting ASD versus non-spectrum best estimate diagnosis. Covariates included the child’s age at the time of administration, IQ and Toddler Module algorithm score. Number of years experience working with children on the autism spectrum was included as a continuous clinician-related predictor (and ranged from 2 years to over 20 years). Results were significant for IQ (β=.05, eβ=1.05, pValidity Study
The goal of the validity study was to create a modified set of codes and algorithm items that could be used with children between 12 and 30 months of age.
Validity of individual items
Following the item revisions and recoding described above, validity was assessed on a final set of 41 items which either showed markedly different distributions across diagnostic groups or which had high clinical or theoretical importance but rare endorsements. Correlation matrices were generated according to diagnostic group using data from unique participants; these included the complete item set as well as verbal and nonverbal mental age, verbal and nonverbal IQ, and chronological age variables. Items which were highly correlated with each other were identified, and some items were eliminated from consideration for the toddler algorithm in order to reduce collinearity (Note: detailed item data will be available in the Toddler Module manual). The strongest association noted between scores and participant characteristics was between “Overall Level of Non-echoed Language†and verbal IQ (r=−.71 across diagnostic groups, n=113), so no items were excluded on this basis.
Exploratory factor analyses were then conducted in Mplus (Muthen & Muthen, 1998) with a focus on ASD participants only. Due to the small sample size, these analyses were not intended to identify a latent class structure for the item data, but rather to provide an assessment of the potential influence of cognitive level and chronological age on these data. Chronological age ceased to load onto any factor when the sample was divided into the two developmental groups (12–20/NV21–30 and V21–30). Verbal mental age did not load onto any factor for either developmental group.
Validity of algorithm
In order to select items for the algorithm, item means and standard deviations were generated across diagnostic groups. The items that best differentiated between diagnoses for the “Unique Participants†and “All Visits†subsets within narrow age/language groups (which were eventually collapsed into the 12–20/NV21–30 and V21–30 groups)were identified. Similarities in diagnostically differential items across the younger (under 21 months) and nonverbal groups, as well as a distinct “best†item set for older verbal toddlers, confirmed the validity of the two developmental groupings used for these analyses. A pool of 17 items was identified as strong candidates for a new Toddler Module algorithm based on their differential distributions across diagnostic group and their relatively low correlations with each other and with chronological age and IQ. Some of these items were new items in the Toddler Module and others had been included in previous Module 1 ADOS algorithms.
Next, best items for each developmental group were summed to generate trial algorithms specifically for the 12–20/NV21–30 and V21–30 groups. Visits missing data from more than 2 algorithm items were excluded from these analyses. Scores of ‘2’ and ‘3’ were collapsed in candidate items following the ADOS convention intended to prevent any one item from exerting undue influence on the total score, and conversely, a score of ‘1’ on the Unusual Eye Contact item was converted to ‘2’ on the algorithms in order to reflect the importance of even subtle differences in eye contact. Receiver Operating Characteristic (ROC) curves (Siegel, Vukicevic, Elliott & Kraemer, 1989) allow sensitivity and specificity percentages to be generated for each total score in a scale. For 12–20/NV21–30 visits, sensitivity and specificity was generated for both trial toddler algorithms as well as the ADOS Module 1, No Words algorithm for “Unique Participants†and “All Visits†subsets of data. For V21–30 visits, ROC curve analyses were run for both trial toddler algorithms and the ADOS Module 1, Some Words algorithm for both “Unique Participants†and “All Visits†subsets. Specificity was evaluated in comparisons of ASD versus non-spectrum participants, and again for ASD versus non-spectrum and typical cases combined, for all possible cutoffs in each of the three possible algorithms. These algorithms were then re-tested by systematically omitting items to ensure that each item contributed to the final differentiations. Within each developmental group, the strongest algorithm out of the three tested was selected by identifying the cutoff score that maximized both sensitivity and specificity across “Unique Participants†and “All Visits†subsets, and that maintained specificity in ASD versus non-spectrum distinctions as well as ASD versus non-spectrum and typical combined. The results are shown in Table 5.
Table 5
Table 5
Sensitivity and specificity of the algorithm cutoffs used with the ADOS-Toddler Module
For children under 21 months and nonverbal toddlers, the same set of items that comprise the ADOS Module 1, No Words algorithm also maximized predictive validity of this measure, though it is important to note that codes and scores associated with items of the same name in the Toddler Module and Module 1 are not identical. A cutoff of 12 on this 12–20/NV21–30 algorithm yielded 91% sensitivity and 91% specificity for ASD versus non-spectrum comparisons of unique participants. This cutoff also maintained sensitivity values at 87% or greater and specificity at 86% or greater when applied to “All Visits†and comparisons of typically developing children (see Table 5 for details). Moreover, the cutoff performed similarly when applied to the 12–20 and NV21–30 groups separately, using both Unique Participants and All Visits samples. All sensitivity and specificity values exceeded 85%, with one exception (75% specificity for ASD versus non-spectrum in the NV21–30 group, based on a cell size of 8).
For verbal toddlers between 21 and 30 months of age, a new algorithm was superior to the Module 1, Some Words algorithm. As shown in Table 5, a cutoff of 10 on this V21–30 algorithm yielded sensitivity of 88% and specificity of 91% in the ASD versus non-spectrum unique participants. Sensitivity was maintained at 81% or greater and specificity at 83% or greater for all other comparisons, with the lowest in these ranges pertaining to “All Visits†repeated comparisons of ASD and Non-spectrum participants. The V21–30 algorithm is comparable in structure to the ADOS revised algorithms, with 14 items organized into Social Affect (SA) and Restricted, Repetitive Behaviors (RRB) domains (see Table 2 for a list of items by domain). In the new V21–30 algorithm, however, only three of these items describe RRBs versus four RRB items in the 12–20/NV21–30 and other revised algorithms across ADOS modules. This difference in maximum RRB total score between the 12–20/NV21–30 and V21–30 algorithms was not theoretically motivated but rather reflects the selection of items that maximized predictive value of the new algorithms in these developmental groups.
To improve clinical utility of this measure, ranges of concern were identified for the new V21–30 algorithm and the 12–20/NV21–30 algorithm used with young or nonverbal toddlers. Using the “Unique Participants†data, three ranges of concern were set for each algorithm, such that at least 95% of children with ASD and no more than about 10% of typically developing children would fall in the two groups suggesting clinical concern (mild-to-moderate and moderate-to-severe). See Table 6 for results.
Table 6
Table 6
Percent of participants falling into ranges of concern by diagnostic group
For both developmental groups, 82% of children with non-ASD developmental delays were accurately assigned to the little-or-no concern range.
Internal consistency of algorithm
In the new V21–30 algorithm, item-total correlations for “All Visits†ranged from .49 (“Response to Nameâ€) to .82 (“Quality of Social Overturesâ€) for the Social Affect domain, and from .18 (“Hand and Finger Mannerismsâ€) to .42 (“Unusual Sensory Interest in Play Material/Personâ€) for the three items comprising the RRB domain (the third being “Unusually Repetitive Interests or Stereotyped Behaviors,†r=.37). Lower correlations within the RRB domain were expected given the heterogeneous nature of these items. Cronbach’s alpha was .90 for the SA domain and .50 for the RRB domain, indicating strong and acceptable internal consistency respectively. Correlations between domain totals and participant characteristics (e.g., chronological age, gender, mental age, and IQ) were evaluated within the “Unique Participants†subset only, because of the known effects of recruitment on the composition of the “All Visits†sample. In the older group of verbal toddlers, domains were correlated at .64 with each other. Across all domain total correlations, noneexceeded −.55 with participant characteristics (between verbal IQ and SA total). Correlations with chronological age did not exceed .48 (with SA total), those with mental age did not exceed −.42 (verbal mental age with SA total), and those with nonverbal IQ did not exceed −.51 (with RRB total).
For the younger or nonverbal children receiving 12–20/NV21–30 algorithm, item-total correlations for “All Visits†ranged from .35 (“Gesturesâ€) to .81 (“Quality of Social Overturesâ€) in the SA domain, and from .14 (“Hand and Finger Mannerismsâ€) to .44 (“Unusually Repetitive Interests or Stereotyped Behaviorsâ€) for the four-item RRB domain. Internal consistency was similar to the older, verbal group findings, with a Cronbach’s alpha of .88 for the SA domain and .50 for the RRB domain. For “Unique Participants†in this developmental group, the domains were correlated at .57 with each other. Across all domain total correlations with participant characteristics, noneexceeded-.58(SA total with verbal mental age). Correlations between domain totals and chronological age did not exceed .34 (with SA total). Correlations with nonverbal mental age did not exceed −.17 (with SA total), those with verbal IQ did not exceed −.38 (with SA total), and those with nonverbal IQ did not exceed −.49 (with SA total).
For both algorithms, SA and RRB domain total scores for “Unique Participants†were significantly higher for the ASD sample than the non-spectrum or typically developing groups (see Table 7). Domain totals for the two non-ASD diagnostic groups did not differ significantly, with the exception of SA scores (non-spectrum mean exceeded typically developing) in the 12–20/NV21–30 group. One-way ANOVA and Tukey test statistics are available from the authors.
Table 7
Table 7
Mean algorithm domain scores by diagnostic group
Reliability Study
Inter-rater reliability of individual items
For reliability analyses, scores indicating that the item was not applicable (generally these were language-related items) were converted to zeros, as is done for algorithm use in the other ADOS modules. Three items (out of a total of 41 items) were either rare or considered particularly valuable in interpreting child behavior (“Stereotyped/Idiosyncratic Use of Words or Phrases,†“Self-Injurious Behavior,†and “Overactivityâ€)had percent agreements exceeding 90 percent but received such a limited range of scores that they were not included in further reliability analyses.
STATA software (StataCorp, 2007) was used to generate weighted kappas for non-unique pairs of raters (i.e., 28 pairs). Kappas between .4 and .74 were considered good, and kappas at or above .75 were considered excellent (Fleiss, 1986). Out of 38 items, 30 weighted kappas were equal to or exceeded .60 (Mkw = .67). The remainder exceeded .45.
Inter-rater item reliability for all items in the protocol was assessed by domain by exploring the percent of exact agreement. Because having reliable ‘3’ scores allows better documentation of variation (which is important in treatment studies), the initial set of analyses retained all scores of ‘0’ to‘3’. Percent agreement between 70% and 79% was considered fair, 80% to 89% was considered good and above 90% was considered excellent (Cicchetti, Volkmar, Klin, & Showalter, 1995). For items on the Toddler Module, even using the extended range of ‘0’ to ‘3’ (which reduces agreement), mean exact (percent) agreement was 84%across all items and rater pairs. Thirty of 41 items had exact agreement at or above 80%, and every item received at least 71%agreement across raters. When considered by domain, agreement for codes related to language and communication was generally good: only three items had reliability that was fair(71%, 74% and 75%). Codes related to reciprocal social interaction were mostly good-to-excellent, with only six items falling in the fair range (75% to 78%). Play and restricted, repetitive behaviors had only one item each in the fair range (78% and 75%, respectively), with all others above 80%. All items in the nonspecific behaviors domain had good or excellent inter-rater reliability.
Because the diagnostic algorithm collapses codes of 2s and 3s (to avoid overly weighting any single item in the overall diagnosis), a second set of exact agreement analyses were conducted, collapsing codes of 2 and 3. Mean exact agreement was 87%. Thirty-five of 41 items had exact agreement above 80%, and no item agreement fell below 71%.
Inter-rater reliability of domain scores and algorithm classifications
Intraclass correlations (ICCs) were computed for protocol total scores, as well as algorithm domain and total scores. Calculations were made using both the 12–20/NV21–30 and V21–30 algorithms. ICCs were as follows: protocol total scores = .96; 12–20/NV21–30 algorithm total = .90; V21–30 algorithm total = .99; 12–20/NV21–30 algorithm SA total = .84;V21–30 algorithm SA total = .99; 12–20/NV21–30 algorithm RRB total = .93; V21–30 algorithm RRB total = .74.
Inter-rater agreement in diagnostic classification using a single cutoff of 12 (i.e., ASD or non-spectrum) was 97% on the 12–20/NV21–30 algorithm. Using the V21–30 algorithm with a single cutoff of 10, inter-rater agreement across diagnostic classifications (i.e., ASD or non-spectrum) was 87%. Inter-rater agreement using the three ranges on the 12–20/NV21–30 algorithm (little-or-no concern: scores less than 10, mild-to-moderate concern: scores of 10 to 13, moderate-to-severe concern: scores of 14 and above) was 70%. On the V21–30 algorithm (little-or-no concern: scores less than 8, mild-to-moderate concern: scores of 8 to 11, moderate-to-severe concern: scores of 12 and above), inter-rater agreement for ranges of concern was 87%.
Test-retest reliability
Test-retest reliability was analyzed using data from all children(n=39)who had two Toddler Module administrations within 2 months. Reliability was evaluated using algorithm subtotal scores across the SA and RRB domains, as well as algorithm total scores. Analyses addressing the 12–20/NV21–30 algorithm, which included 31 participants, yielded high test-retest ICCs for the SA total (.83), the RRB total (.75), and the algorithm total score (.86). The mean absolute difference across the two evaluations was 0.90 points (SD = 3.14) for SA, 0.39 points (SD = 1.54) for RRBs and 1.29 points (SD = 3.55) for the algorithm total score. Out of the 31 children, 24(77%)were classified consistently across the two evaluations (using the single cutoff of 10 on the algorithm). Out of the 7 participants who shifted between non-spectrum and ASD classification, 3 initially missed the cutoff and then met the cutoff on the second evaluation, while 4 moved from meeting the cutoff to failing to meet. Using the three ranges of concern, 23 (74%) children were classified within the same range across evaluations. Of the 8 participants who shifted between ranges of concern, 1 shifted from the greater level of concern to the lesser one. Seven shifted from little-or-no concern to a concern range or vice-versa (2 from little-or-no concern to mild-to-moderate concern, 4 from mild-to-moderate concern to no concern, and 1 from moderate-to-severe concern to little-or-no concern).
Data for 8 participants who received the V21–30 algorithm twice within two months indicated similarly high ICCs for the SA total (.94), the RRB total (.60), and the algorithm total score (.95). There was a mean absolute difference across the two evaluations of 0.63 points(SD = 2.13) for algorithm total scores, 0.38 points (SD = 2.77)for the SA total, and 0.25 points (SD = 1.04) for the RRB total. Using the single cutoff of 10, 2 children shifted classifications across evaluations (1 shifting from meeting cutoffs to failing to meet, the other vice-versa) and 6 retained the same classification. Similarly, 5 out of the 8 children remained in the same range of concern across both administrations. Of the remaining 3 children, 1 increased from mild-to-moderate to moderate-to-severe concern, 1 moved from mild-to-moderate to little-or-no concern and the other shifted from little-or-noconcern to mild-to-moderate concern.
Go to:
Discussion
The Toddler Module contributes a new module to the existing ADOS and permits the use of this standardized instrument with children under 30 months of age. It includes three core areas of observation, namely, language and communication, reciprocal social interaction, play and stereotyped/restricted behaviors or interests. Algorithm scores have acceptable internal consistency and excellent inter-rater and test-retest reliability. The algorithm, using both the formal cutoff and the ranges of concern, has excellent diagnostic validity for ASD versus non-spectrum conditions. Children who receive the Toddler Module should have a nonverbal age equivalent of at least 12 months and be walking independently. If a child has not yet attained all of these milestones, Toddler Module results may be elevated due to developmental factors and must be interpreted with care.
The lower chronological age limit for the Toddler Module is proposed to be 12 months. This is estimated based on the nonverbal mental age requirement of 12 months and the increased observation of more children on the spectrum performing at age expectations (Chakrabarti & Fombonne, 2001). However, the current sample included only one child under the age of 15 months who met this nonverbal mental age criteria. Therefore, the present investigation validated the proposed algorithms only down to 15 months of chronological age. It is clear that the Toddler Module tasks and items are appropriate for children in the age range. It is also apparent that in chronological ages under 15 months, the algorithm had good specificity in this sample (due to the higher numbers of 12 to 15 month olds in the non-spectrum and typically developing groups). However, the sensitivity of the proposed algorithm has not yet been established for children with ASD who (a) have nonverbal mental ages of at least 12 months and (b) are between 12 and 15 months of chronological age. This will need to be addressed in future investigations in order for the lower chronological age cutoff to be confirmed.
As with other modules of the ADOS, the Toddler Module algorithm should be interpreted cautiously and in conjunction with other sources of information. Use of the algorithm ranges should be one element of a comprehensive diagnostic assessment, in which the final diagnostic decision must be made using the best judgment of the clinician. This is particularly important when evaluating very young children, for whom the lines of typical and atypical development can be very unclear and for whom behavior can change over a few months. Moreover, differential diagnosis can be especially challenging in toddlers because symptoms may emerge gradually. An attempt has been made to structure the Toddler Module algorithm in a manner which – as much as is possible – accommodates these observations by generating ranges of concern rather than strict classifications. In addition, because research has indicated that early specific ASD diagnoses (autism and PDD-NOS versus ASD) have questionable stability in younger populations, the algorithms provide only one research cutoff for all ASD.
The single cutoffs proposed for the new algorithms should be interpreted in a fashion consistent with the ADOS: “an individual who meets or exceeds the cutoffs … has scored within the range of a high proportion of participants with [ASD] who have similar levels of expressive language and deficits in social behavior and in the use of speech and gesture as part of social interaction†(Lord et al., 2000, p. 220). However, in order to warrant an ASD diagnosis, the individual must otherwise exhibit behaviors consistent with the criteria as outlined in formal diagnostic criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). That is, it is possible for a child to meet a cutoff and not receive a formal diagnosis of ASD according to clinical judgment. Conversely, it is also possible for a child to score below the cutoff and for a clinician to judge that the child does meet formal criteria for an ASD diagnosis. Some aspects of the algorithm scores (i.e., negative association with early verbal scores) highlight the importance of thoughtful clinical interpretation of algorithm results, because certain features of the child which are non-specific to ASD (like early language delay) may elevate scores. Because verbal ability in this study was defined by MSEL (Mullen, 1995) scores, and – as with other measures – the early MSEL scores are heavily biased to social communication (e.g., “recognizes own name†and “plays gesture/language gameâ€), the correlations between Toddler Module scores and early verbal ability scores seemed inevitable, though a clearer separation between ADOS scores and eventual language ability would be ideal.
The ranges of concern which are incorporated into the algorithm are intended to reflect the diagnostic uncertainty that is often faced when evaluating very young children, whether because of developmental variability or confounding conditions (such as global developmental delay or early language impairment). Nevertheless, by expanding the number of categories from two diagnostic groupings (ASD and non-spectrum) to three ranges of concern (little-or-no, mild-to-moderate, moderate-to-severe), more variation would be expected. Thus, the ranges are intended primarily as “sign-posts†along a continuous range of scores that show excellent stability in intra-class correlations, across raters and re-assessments several months later. Scores falling into the little-or-no concern range suggest that the child demonstrates no more behaviors associated with ASD than children in this age range who do not have ASD. Generally, scores which fall into the mild-to-moderate range should be considered an indicator of behaviors likely to be consistent with an ASD. Children whose scores fall into this range should receive further ASD-specific evaluation and follow-up in the next several months, including ongoing monitoring of cognitive and language development, as well as ASD symptoms. Note that a minority of children with non-spectrum conditions and typical development also scored in this range, so there is considerable heterogeneity within it. In contrast, algorithm scores falling into the moderate-to-severe range of concern were strongly consistent with an eventual diagnosis of ASD (with only 3–6% false positives). Regardless, whether using the research-oriented cutoff or the clinically-oriented ranges of concern, the onus is on the examiner to interpret behaviors and scores within the broader developmental and assessment context. In cases of diagnostic uncertainty, it is important to be clear with parents (particularly of very young children) about the importance of ongoing monitoring of child development and thorough follow-up.
The importance of the algorithm and its items may lead ADOS administrators to ask why additional codes are necessary. There are two primary purposes for including codes in the ADOS which are not algorithm items. First, the present investigation is an initial attempt to generate a research and clinical tool. New information from larger, independent investigations may result in improved algorithms using a different set of items, as has been the case for the ADOS (Gotham et al., 2007; Gotham et al., 2008). Second, the non-algorithm items describe important aspects of ASD and may characterize the strengths and weaknesses of individual children. Changes in non-algorithm items may provide valuable information concerning response to treatment and, more speculatively, different etiological subtypes or patterns of behavior.
The young age of the children receiving the Toddler Module means that the examiner may face some additional issues in interpreting ADOS results. Specifically, some infants and toddlers may be very uncomfortable in the evaluation context, where they are faced with an unknown adult, unfamiliar toys, and a novel clinic or laboratory setting. The examiner must, therefore, gauge whether behavior observed in the ADOS context is representative of behavior in other settings. This is especially important if something about the ADOS assessment – an unskilled examiner, the absence of a familiar caregiver, cultural differences in expected child behavior – might suggest that the child’s behavior is “offâ€. Fortunately, because the Toddler Module requires that (barring unique circumstances, such as children recently placed in foster care) a familiar caregiver is always present in the room, the examiner should get feedback from the caregiver about whether the child’s behavior during the ADOS was representative of day-to-day interactions. If something about the ADOS administration indicates that the observation did not capture the child’s every-day behavior, the scores should be interpreted accordingly and more information should be sought through a home observation or a repeated assessment.
In addition to the above child-related factors, there are important examiner-related factors which must be considered when using the Todder Module. All examiners in the present investigation had at least two years of intensive experience working with young children at risk for and identified with ASD. Furthermore, all examiners had participated extensively – either through consensus discussions or supervision – in generating early differential diagnoses. This high level of experience in working with the relevant population is extremely important, in terms of both clinical skill and the validity of clinical judgment. Although the current study did not find an association between degree of clinical experience and final diagnostic judgments, previous projects have reported that limited experience is associated with lower clinician agreement for specific spectrum diagnoses (Stone, Lee, Ashford, & Brissie, 1999). As previously stated, information obtained from the Toddler Module should be only one component of a diagnostic decision. Nevertheless, it is extremely important that the measure be used by individuals who have sufficient clinical experience to appropriately interpret the observations and algorithm results.
Results and observations from the Toddler Module may be useful beyond the diagnostic context. Parents, intervention providers and teachers often report that the strengths and difficulties noted during the administration can help in understanding an individual child and developing programming goals. Therefore, clinicians should make a concerted effort to thoroughly explain the key observations in behavioral terms (rather than simply in terms of scores and cutoffs), describing which behaviors were noted and which were less consistent or absent. When appropriate, examiners should generate suitable recommendations based on the ADOS observations which can be applied to educational and treatment plans at home and at school.
The predictive validity of very early diagnosis (under 30 months) is a question currently being addressed by many investigators (Chawarska, Klin, Paul & Volkmar, 2007; Landa & Garrett-Mayer, 2006; Wetherby et al., 2004; Zwaigenbaum et al., 2005). The focus of the Toddler Module development is to provide a standardized method of quantifying descriptions of behaviors that correspond to experienced clinicians’ best estimate clinical diagnosis of ASD at a given point in time. The Toddler Module provides information with good to excellent internal consistency and inter-rater reliability for items, domains and research diagnostic categories. Stability across raters within clinical ranges was good for older, verbal children but less good for the nonverbal and younger children. Across time, about three-quarters of children remained in the same clinical range of concern for both algorithms, and slightly fewer remained in the same diagnostic category. Thus, variations both in rater and in time do make a difference in a child’s outcome on the Toddler Module. Follow-up studies of the long-term predictive value of these scores will be critical in determining the extent to which they, and other early measures of diagnostic risk, predict outcome and response to treatment. In the meantime, consideration of scores as continuous dimensions and as one marker (along with other measures) of relative risk of ASD and need for follow-up seems most appropriate. In research, the diagnostic categories may help in standardizing assessments across studies and establishing replicable criteria for study inclusion. Again, however, algorithm classification should be considered in the context of other information.
There are some limitations to the present investigation. The sample size is small and did not permit very fine-grained age groupings. Of particular importance is the limited number of NS children in the 21–30 V group and the limited number of very young children with ASD. Furthermore, the ASD sample was considerably larger than the comparison samples, which may have affected the sensitivity and specificity of the cut-offs. It was also noted that many of the children in the ASD sample had age-level nonverbal abilities. Although a higher-functioning sample (versus a more impaired one) may better approximate the cohort of children currently receiving diagnoses (Chakrabarti & Fombonne, 2001), it provides less information about symptom overlap between ASD and other non-spectrum conditions in children with marked intellectual disabilities. All of these factors may have affected the observed results and need to be addressed in additional samples to confirm the validity of the currently proposed measure guidelines (e.g., use for children under 15 months of age) and algorithm construction.
Test-retest reliability was evaluated over the course of up to 2 months (rather than over the course of several days) and may be confounded by developmental changes. In addition, it is important to acknowledge that evaluation and diagnosis were not completely independent processes because the administration of the ADOS was part of standard practice, although diagnosis was independent of algorithm results. Finally, a cross-validation sample is required to test the algorithm cut-offs (and their associated sensitivity and specificity). It will be important to address these concerns, as well as broader questions such as calibration (using algorithm scores as continuous measurements of severity) through replication in future independent studies.
In sum, the Toddler Module is a new, standardized module intended to extend the application of the ADOS to children as young as 12 months of age who have nonverbal mental ages of at least 12 months. It is appropriate for use with children up to the age of 30 months or until children acquire phrase speech. Replication of the psychometric results reported here with larger, more diverse samples of children with early-appearing, non-spectrum conditions as well as with ASD is crucial, as are follow-up studies that provide information about predictive validity. We hope that researchers and clinicians alike find it a useful tool in supporting families and children with autism spectrum disorders and advancing our understanding of these conditions.
Go to:
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by NRSA F31MH73210-02 from the National Institute of Mental Health to Rhiannon Luyster, as well as grants MH57167 and MH066469 from the National Institute of Mental Health and HD 35482-01 from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, and funding from the Simons Foundation to Catherine Lord. Support was also provided by a grant from the Department of Education to Amy Wetherby. We thank Andrea Cohan, Christina Corsello, Pamela Dixon Thomas, Lee Anne Green Snyder, Alexandra Hessenius, Marisela Huerta, Lindsay Jackson, Jennifer Kleinke, Fiona Miller, Rebecca Niehus and Dorothy Ramos for their assistance in data collection. We would also like to express our gratitude to the families and children in the Toddlers study, the Word Learning project, and the First Words project.
Go to:
Footnotes
Some of the data from this paper were previously presented at the 2006 International Meeting for Autism Research (IMFAR) in Montreal, the 2nd World Autism Congress & Exhibition in Cape Town, South Africa, the 2007 Society for Research in Child Development conference in Boston, Massachusetts and at the 2008 IMFAR in London, England.
The Toddler Module (Lord, Luyster, Gotham & Guthrie) is currently in press at Western Psychological Services. The authors of this paper received no royalties from the Toddler Module while it was under development, nor did Drs. Lord or Risi receive royalties for use of any other ADOS modules, due to an agreement with the University of Michigan such that all profits from the authors’ use of the measure are donated to charity. The authors of the Toddler Module will receive royalties upon its publication.
11Inquires about Toddler Module protocols, kits and training should be directed to Western Psychological Services.
Go to:
Contributor Information
Rhiannon Luyster, University of Michigan Autism and Communication Disorders Center.
Katherine Gotham, University of Michigan Autism and Communication Disorders Center.
Whitney Guthrie, University of Michigan Autism and Communication Disorders Center.
Mia Coffing, University of Michigan Autism and Communication Disorders Center.
Rachel Petrak, University of Michigan Autism and Communication Disorders Center.
Karen Pierce, University of California – San Diego.
Somer Bishop, University of Michigan Autism and Communication Disorders Center.
Amy Esler, University of Michigan Autism and Communication Disorders Center.
Vanessa Hus, University of Michigan Autism and Communication Disorders Center.
Rosalind Oti, University of Michigan Autism and Communication Disorders Center.
Jennifer Richler, University of Michigan Autism and Communication Disorders Center.
Susan Risi, University of Michigan Autism and Communication Disorders Center.
Catherine Lord, University of Michigan Autism and Communication Disorders Center.
Go to:
References
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4. Washington, DC: Author; 1994.
Behne T, Carpenter M, Call J, Tomasello M. Unwilling versus unable: Infants’ understanding of intentional action. Developmental Psychology. 2005;41(2):328–337. [PubMed]
Bohlin G, Hagekull B. Stranger wariness and sociability in the early years. Infant Behavior and Development. 1993;16(1):53–67.
Bryson SE, Zwaigenbaum L, McDermott C, Rombough V, Brian J. The Autism Observational Scale for Infants: Scale development and reliability data. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2008;38(4):731–738. [PubMed]
Chakrabarti S, Fombonne E. Pervasive developmental disorders in preschool children. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2001;285(24):3093–3099. [PubMed]
Charman T, Taylor E, Drew A, Cockerill H, Brown J, Baird G. Outcome at 7 years of children diagnosed with autism at age 2: Predictive validity of assessments conducted at 2 and 3 years of age and pattern of symptom change over time. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2005;46(5):500–513. [PubMed]
Chawarska K, Klin A, Paul R, Volkmar F. Autism spectrum disorder in the second year: Stability and change in syndrome expression. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2007;48(2):128–138. [PubMed]
Chawarska K, Paul R, Klin A, Hannigen S, Dichtel LE, Volkmar F. Parental recognition of developmental problems in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2007;37(1):62–72. [PubMed]
Cicchetti D, Volkmar F, Klin A, Showalter D. Diagnosing autism using ICD-10 criteria: A comparison of neural networks and standard multivariate procedures. Child Neuropsychology. 1995;1:26–37.
DeGiacomo A, Fombonne E. Parental recognition of developmental abnormalities in autism. European Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1998;7:131–136.
DiLavore P, Lord C, Rutter M. Pre-linguistic Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (PLADOS) Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1995;25(4):355–379. [PubMed]
Fleiss J. Reliability of measurements. In: Fleiss J, editor. The design and analysis of clinical experiments. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 1986. pp. 2–31.
Gotham K, Risi S, Pickles A, Lord C. The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS): Revised algorithms for improved diagnostic validity. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2007;37(4):613–627. [PubMed]
Gotham K, Risi S, Dawson G, Tager-Flusberg H, Joseph R, Carter A, Hepburn S, McMahon W, Rodier P, Hyman SL, Sigman M, Rogers S, Landa R, Spence MA, Osann K, Flodman P, Volkmar F, Hollander E, Buxbaum J, Pickles A, Lord C. A replication of the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) revised algorithms. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2008;47(6):642–651. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Kleinman JM, Ventola PE, Pandey J, Verbalis AD, Barton M, Hodgson S, Green J, Dumont-Mathieu T, Robins DL, Fein D. Diagnostic stability in very young children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2008;38(4):606–615. [PubMed]
Landa R, Garrett-Mayer E. Development in infants with autism spectrum disorders: A prospective study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2006;47(6):629–638. [PubMed]
Lord C, Risi S, DiLavore P, Shulman C, Thurm A, Pickles A. Autism from two to nine. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2006;63(6):694–701. [PubMed]
Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EH, Leventhal BL, DiLavore P, Pickles A, Rutter M. The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule –Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2000;30(3):205–223. [PubMed]
Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore P, Risi S. Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services; 1999.
Lord C, Rutter M, Goode S, Heemsbergen J, Jordan H, Mawhood L, Schopler E. Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule: A standardized observation of communicative and social behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1989;19:185–212. [PubMed]
Lord C, Shulman C, DiLavore P. Regression and word loss in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2004;45(5):936–955. [PubMed]
Mullen E. Mullen Scales of Early Learning. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service, Inc; 1995.
Muthen LK, Muthen BO. M-plus user’s guide. Los Angeles: Muthen and Muthen; 1998.
National Research Council. Educating children with autism. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 2001.
Phillips W, Baron-Cohen S, Rutter M. The role of eye contact in goal detection: Evidence from normal infants and children with autism or mental handicap. Development and Psychopathology. 1992;4:375–383.
Risi S, Lord C, Gotham K, Corsello C, Chrysler C, Szatmari P, Cook EH, Jr, Leventhal BL, Pickles A. Combining information from multiple sources in the diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2006;45(9):1094. [PubMed]
Rutter M, Le Couteur A, Lord C. Autism Diagnosic Interview -Revised (ADI-R) Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services; 2003.
Siegel B, Vukicevic J, Elliott G, Kraemer H. The use of signal detection theory to assess DSM-III-R criteria for autistic disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1989;28:542–548. [PubMed]
Sroufe LA. Wariness of strangers and the study of infant development. Child Development. 1977;48:1184–1199.
StataCorp. Stata Statistical Software: Release 10. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP; 2007.
Stone W, Coonrod EE, Turner LM, Pozdol SL. Psychometric properties of the STAT for early autism screening. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2004;34(6):691–701. [PubMed]
Stone W, Lee E, Ashford L, Brissie J. Can Autism be Diagnosed Accurately in Children Under Three Years? Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry. 1999;20(2):219–226. [PubMed]
Turner L, Stone W. Variability in outcome for children with an ASD diagnosis at age 2. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2007;48(8):793–802. [PubMed]
Turner L, Stone WL, Pozdol S, Coonrod EE. Follow-up of children with autism spectrum disorders from age 2 to age 9. Autism. 2006;10(3):243–265. [PubMed]
Wetherby A. Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales Developmental Profile, Preliminary Normed Edition. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing; 2001.
Wetherby A, Woods J, Allen L, Cleary J, Dickinson H, Lord C. Early indicators of autism spectrum disorders in the second year of life. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2004;34(5):473–493. [PubMed]
Zwaigenbaum L, Bryson S, Rogers T, Roberts W, Brian J, Szatmari P. Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of life. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience. 2005;23:143–152. [PubMed]
A nasal spray – Oxytocin – helps improve autistic children’s social connections. That’s the essence of a recent Yale study. So where do parents get oxytocin nasal spray? Can it be prescribed by the child’s pediatrician? More to follow.
_______________
“Preliminary results from an ongoing, large-scale study by Yale School of Medicine researchers shows that oxytocin — a naturally occurring substance produced in the brain and throughout the body— increased brain function in regions that are known to process social information in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders (ASD).
“A Yale Child Study Center research team that includes postdoctoral fellow Ilanit Gordon and Kevin Pelphrey, the Harris Associate Professor of Child Psychiatry and Psychology, will present the results on Saturday, May 19 at 3 p.m. at the International Meeting for Autism Research.
“Our findings provide the first, critical steps toward devising more effective treatments for the core social deficits in autism, which may involve a combination of clinical interventions with an administration of oxytocin,†said Gordon. “Such a treatment approach will fundamentally improve our understanding of autism and its treatment.â€
Social-communicative dysfunctions are a core characteristic of autism, a neurodevelopmental disorder that can have an enormous emotional and financial burden on the affected individual, their families, and society.
Gordon said that while a great deal of progress has been made in the field of autism research, there remain few effective treatments and none that directly target the core social dysfunction. Oxytocin has recently received attention for its involvement in regulating social abilities because of its role in many aspects of social behavior and social cognition in humans and other species.
To assess the impact of oxytocin on the brain function, Gordon and her team conducted a first-of-its-kind, double-blind, placebo-controlled study on children and adolescents aged 7 to 18 with ASD. The team members gave the children a single dose of oxytocin in a nasal spray and used functional magnetic resonance brain imaging to observe its effect.The team found that oxytocin increased activations in brain regions known to process social information. Gordon said these brain activations were linked to tasks involving multiple social information processing routes, such as seeing, hearing, and processing information relevant to understanding other people.
Other authors on the study include Randi H. Bennett, Brent C. vander Wyk, James F. Leckman, and Ruth Feldman.
Intriguing as a possible early sign of autism: Lag in motor development (control of head and neck) found in infants who are more likely to develop autism. This study needs to be replicated before the public puts much weight on this finding.
This is a simple test that any mother can do at home. Any suspected delays can lead to early intervention by Early Start services. Parents who suspect motor delays can seek assistance from the Early Start program funded by the federal government.
“Typically, red flags that might lead to an autism diagnosis are issues with social and communicative traits, such as avoiding eye contact or not playing with others. But Dr. Rebecca Landa, the study’s author and director of the Center for Autism and Related Disorders at Kennedy Krieger Institute in Baltimore, says certain disruptions in a child’s motor development may provide important clues.
“For the study, researchers assessed infants in a simple “pull-to-sit” task that measures posture control by firmly – yet carefully – pulling a child’s arms from a position of lying flat on his/her side back into a sitting position (as seen in the videos below). Typically infants achieve this type of posture control by the time they are four months old.
“In one experiment, researchers gave this task to 40 infants who were considered to be genetically high-risk for the disorder because a sibling has autism. They researchers were looking specifically at “head lag” – the inability to control head posture – at 6, 14, 24 and 30 months of age.
“The researchers found 90 percent of subjects eventually diagnosed with autism exhibited head lags as infants, and 54 percent of kids who met social and communication delays criteria exhibited head lag, while 35 percent of children who did not meet that criteria exhibited the lag.
“In a second experiment, Landa and her team examined only six month olds at a single point in time to check for head lag, and found 75 percent of the high risk infants displayed head lag compared with 33 percent of low-risk infants, further emphasizing that head lag is more common in infants that may develop autism.
Landa’s study is to be presented at the International Meeting for Autism Research on May 17 in Toronto.
“While previous research shows that motor impairments are linked to social and communication deficits in older children with autism, the field is just starting to examine this in younger children,” she said in an Institute news release. “Our initial research suggests that motor delays may have an important impact on child development.”
“If some parents try the test at home and are worried, Landa emphasized to The Baltimore Sun that a head lag at six months does not mean a child is definitely going to have autism, but rather is a potential sign that a pediatrician should explore further. http://www.baltimoresun.com/health/blog/bal-poh-autism-test-for-infants,0,1502094.story
“We don’t want to scare parents,” she said. “If I go to the doctor because I’m having problems with balance, he’s not going to assume I have a brain tumor. When a baby shows a head lag there are so many other things it can be. But this is a very real indicator of something wrong with development and easy things can be done to help.”
Dr. Alycia Halladay, director of environmental research for the advocacy and research group Autism Speaks, told WebMD that the findings are “intriguing” but a head lag’s diagnostic value remains uncertain.
“The first step is to replicate these outcomes in larger studies in multiple sites,” she said.
The study adds to recent research aimed at diagnosing autism at an early age. A recent study found differences in nerve connections seen in infants’ brain scans might signal autism, CBS News medical correspondent Dr. Jon LaPook reported.
About 1 in 88 children has autism, according to recent government estimates.
Hearing loss in babies has huge effects on their general development: hearing loss impacts language acquisition, speech, psycho-social well being and overall learning.
Research shows that the critical time to stimulate the auditory and brain pathways is during the first six (6) months of your child’s life. So pay particular attention during your child’s first six months that he or she continues to hear normally.
The good news is that children with all degrees of hearing loss — who receive appropriate interventions prior to 6 months of age– can obtain speech and language skills comparable to their normal hearing peers when age 3 years.
What parents can do:
Check and re-check that your baby’s hearing remains normal. Visit your pediatrician for screenings as your doctor recommends. Research recommends hearing screening every 2 months until age one year — and every three months until age two.
Keep this in mind:
Even mild hearing loss can significantly interfere with the reception of spoken language and educational performance. Research shows that children with one ear hearing loss are ten (10) times as likely to be held back at least one year compared to children with normal hearing.
Many children are affected with ear infections: Chronic otis media (ear infections) affects 5 – 30% of children age 6 to 11 years and can persist 4 – 5 months with or without medical interventions.
Watch for possible symptoms of hearing and ear problems. Does your child: Tug at his/her ear; turn side of head towards parent; appear inattentive; strain when listening; make frequent mistakes following directions; day dreams; tend to isolate; tire easily; talk too loudly or too softly; have a speech problem; appear passive.
Does your child appear to have pain in their ear? Do you see redness or drainage from the ear?
Methods to assess for hearing loss in young children:
Otoacoustic Emissions Technology (OAE)
Otoacoustic Emissions is a hearing test that uses a small probe inserted into the external ear to introduce a sound stimulus (series of beeps) and measures the response sound, like an echo, emitted by the inner ear (cochlea) of a normal hearing person. The cochlea of a person with a hearing loss greater than 25-30 dB does not emit a sound in response to a sound stimulus.
Many studies have shown that screening children 0 – 3 years of age may be beneficial with OAEs. The OAE technology is very good for children who are unable to respond to a sound by raising their hand or dropping a toy in a bucket to indicate a response to the stimulus.
Children with developmental delays [possible autistic spectrum issues, possible intellectual disability] may not understand or often refuse to follow simple directions. OAE may be a useful screening for children with developmental delays.
Note that OAE may not detect mild hearing loss (20 dB to 40 dB) which may affect performance in school. The gold standard for screening children over age three is with a pure tone audiometer conducted by properly trained personnel.
To summarize: Parents can greatly reduce possible hearing loss in babies and young children with appropriate interventions. Know why its so important that children hear normally. Get medical attention immediately when young children appear to have difficulties with hearing normally.
Take children for routine screenings every 2 months during first year or as your pediatrician recommends. Watch for signs of possible ear infection and behaviors that indicate difficulties with hearing. Knowledge, parent involvement and appropriate medical interventions to improve hearing are particularly crucial during your child’s first six months. Parents can do a lot to reduce hearing loss in babies and young children.
In CA, children with developmental delays are typically referred for Early Start services. Early Start services are provided through the government from birth until the child turns three years old.
Roughly 70% of the children referred for Early Start services in CA are due to speech delays.
And, a very important medical questions is: Can this child hear normally? If a child applying for Early Start services cannot hear normally different medical interventions are needed to address hearing loss.
In Santa Cruz County assessment for Early Start services is done via various vendors. These vendors provide assessment reports concerning the overall development of the child/ applicant for Early Start services.
Whether or not the applicant for Early Start services can hear normally is vital to determining the appropriate kind of services to provide to the child.
Any parent who applies for Early Start services in Santa Cruz County should check with the vendor and whoever receives the assessment report and inquire about their child’s hearing abilities. Was the child’s hearing abilities screened? Can the child hear normally? And if the child does not hear normally, what appropriate medical services are recommended?
Remember the first sentences of this post: Hearing loss in young babies has profound effects on the general development of a child. And, parents can help in many ways so that their child hears and develops normally.
Parents can use simple techniques to help children with ADHD and/ or Autistic Spectrum Disorders (ASD) to be happier and achieve goals using cognitive-behavioral maethods, small rewards and practise, practise, practise the STEPS to each goal. DrCameronJackson@gmail.com
Some simple techniques can assist children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and/ or autistic spectrum disorder (ASD)to be happier and achieve appropriate goals. These techniques are easy for parents and teachers to implement.
Of course a child is not a car but think of the following analogy:
Every car to get to a destination needs a driver. Parents can assist children to get into the driver’s seat and drive their ‘car’ to appropriate ‘destinations’.
Here’s how to assist children who have difficulties with focus, attention, concentration, ‘executive functioning’ or ‘working memory’ difficulties. Executive functioning and working memory are words used by school psychologists. What the parent sees is a child who can’t seem to figure out what comes first and appear disorganized.
What to do: Get a pad of 8 by 12 inch lined paper.
On one per sheet of paper —
1) Write at the top, a Goal that parents/teachers want the child to achieve.
2) Write out all the Steps required to accomplish the Goal.
3) Order the steps from first to last using KISS (Keep It Simple, Simple)
4) Figure out an appropriate Reward for child accomplishing the steps.
Set it up so child can get partial reward for partial completion.
5) Decide the number of Days child needs to complete the Goal.
6) Draw up a Graph with Days across Top of Page and Goals listed on left.
Here’s an example of a Behavior Plan for a child who shows symptoms of both ADHD and ASD:
Gina is 8 years old and was exposed in utero to illegal drugs and alcohol. Her biological mother lost parental rights and Gina has been in foster care since age two. Since age four, Gina has received County Mental Health therapy and medication to assist with focus, attention and concentration. The therapy provided by County Mental Health focuses only on helping Gina to label emotions and better express her emotions appropriately.
Gina’s foster mother and her teacher agree that Gina’s adaptive functioning abilities are considerably below what they expect for her age. Her foster mother wants Gina to 1) wash her face & brush her teeth; 2) make her bed; 3) put toys in basket in her room; 4) set the table with utensils and plates before dinner.
Goals:
Monday Tuesday Wednesday Thursday Friday Saturday Sunday
Wash face & clean teeth
Make bed
Put toys in basket
Set table with utensils & plates
Spiritual/ religious beliefs need to be integral to a successful Behavioral Plan.
For example, Gina’s foster mother goes to church and rests on Sunday. Therefore the behavior plan is for six days a week and no work on Sunday, their day of rest. Six days a week time four goals = 24 Goals for Gina to achieve. For each Goal achieved she earns one (1) dime. During the week she puts stickers on the sheet and gets her motivators (dimes) which she puts into a plastic, clear jar. That way she can see exactly what she has earned towards a new toy on the weekend. Her foster mother believes Gina can find age appropriate toys for around $2.50 to $3.00 a toy.
What makes a Behavioral Plan successful?
The answer is simple: practice and more practice so the Steps are as simple as possible and the child gets rewarded for partial and then complete finishing the Goal.
For example, Gina when we started could not make her bed said her foster mom. Well, the cover was too big and too heavy for a thin, small 8 year old to move around. Thus, by simplifying and making a bed simply tossing a light duvet (down comforter) on a twin bed and putting the pillow in place — then making a bed by an 8 year old is simple and possible.
A second example:
Gina has never set the table for dinner. Part of the reason is that the plates are too high for her to reach and they are china easily breakable. Also, the utensils are not easy to get to. This is easily solved by moving plastic plates and smaller folks and spoons to one drawer that the child can reach. The point is to set it up so the child can successfully achieve the Goals.
Overall point: Parents can set simple ‘destinations’ for their child to drive their ‘car’ towards and by keeping the steps simple and practising the steps again and again their child can achieve those destinations to goals that parents set.
Very important: the hugs and ‘You did great!’ and ‘Keep trying!’ are as important and at times more important than any reward system. Parents – whether biological parents or foster parents – are the fire to ignite important, life long change in children.
Send comments to DrCameronJackson@gmail.com
Parents can download for free a research based manual “A Parent’s Guide to Evidence-Based Practice and Autism.” This manual is co-authored by parents with autistic children and professionals.
If possible I will put a copy up on the Autism page for Monterey Bay Forum. Go to http://www.nationalautismcenter.org to download a copy.
More information below:
National Autism Center
_____________
” Parents and caregivers everywhere are eager for credible, research-based information on the most effective treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD).
To address this need, the National Autism Center has released its newest manual, “A Parent’s Guide to Evidence-Based Practice and Autism.” The Center is a nonprofit organization dedicated to supporting effective, evidence-based treatment approaches for individuals with ASD.
The 134-page manual focuses on helping parents as they make decisions about how to best help children with ASD reach their full potential. It begins with a review of the autism spectrum, symptoms, and co-occurring conditions, and identifies and describes effective treatments. Other topics include the importance of professional judgment, the role of family preferences and values in the decision-making process, and factors to consider when choosing a team of professionals to help their child.
“Finding information about autism is easy. It is much more difficult to find reliable information that has withstood the rigors of science, is comprehensive in scope, and is accessible and easy to read,” says Hanna C. Rue, Ph.D., BCBA-D, Director of Evidence-based Practice for the National Autism Center and one of the manual’s authors. “Even for a trained professional, sorting through the clutter to find information that is most relevant to a child’s needs is a complicated and challenging process.”
One of the features that make this manual different from most others is that it was co-authored by professionals and parents of children with autism. “As professionals, we think about treatment from an entirely different perspective than a parent,” Dr. Rue continues. “We felt it was critical that the parent experience be reflected in every aspect of the manual.”
“To have a trusted resource that you can go to for reliable information, or to check your own gut instinct, is essential for any parent of a child with autism,” explains Janet Amorello, one of the manual’s parent experts. “My hope is that this manual will help families assess the options and obtain services that ultimately result in a better outcome for their child.”
The manual is the latest in a series of publications by the National Autism Center. Visit the Center’s website to download a free copy, watch a video, or learn more.
About the National Autism Center
The National Autism Center is May Institute’s Center for the Promotion of Evidence-based Practice. It is dedicated to serving children and adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) by providing reliable information, promoting best practices, and offering comprehensive resources for families, practitioners, and communities.
The Center works to shape public policy concerning ASD and its treatment through the development and dissemination of national standards of practice.
For more information, please call 877.313.3833 or visit www.nationalautismcenter.org
Contact: Susan G. Lauermann, APR(781) 437-1257slauermann@nationalautismcenter.org
About one-third of children with autistic spectrum issues also have epilepsy. Diet does help with both per study discussed below.
Roughly 25-35% of individuals with autism eventually develop seizures and many of the remainder have subclinical seizure-like brain activity. However, little is known about which traditional epilepsy treatments and commonly used non-traditional alternative treatments are effective for treating seizures or epilepsy in children and adults with autism spectrum disorder.
A study just published in BMC Pediatrics by Dr. Richard E. Frye from the University of Texas in Houston and Dr. James B. Adams from the Arizona State University in Tempe has now provided insight into which traditional and non-traditional medical treatments are most beneficial for individuals with autism spectrum disorder and seizures. These researchers surveyed 733 parents of children with autism spectrum disorder and seizures, epilepsy and/or subclinical seizure-like brain activity to rate the effectiveness of 25 traditional and 20 non-traditional medical treatments on seizures. The survey also assessed the effect of those treatments on other symptoms (sleep, communication, behavior, attention and mood)and side effects.
Overall, anti-epileptic drugs were reported by parents to improve seizures but worsened other symptoms. Overall, non-antiepileptic drugs were perceived to improve other symptoms but did not improve seizures to the same extent as the anti-epileptic drugs. Four anti-epileptic drugs, valproic acid, lamotrigine, levetiracetam and ethosuximide, were reported to improve seizures the most and, on average, have little positive or negative effect on other symptoms. Certain traditional non-anti-epileptic drug treatments, particularly the ketogenic diet, were perceived to improve both seizures and other symptoms.“The information gained from this study will help physicians more effectively manage children with autism spectrum disorder and seizures,” says Dr Frye.
Prof. Adams states that, “This study suggests that several non-traditional treatments, such as special diets (ketogenic, Atkins, and gluten-free, casein-free), are worth further investigation as adjunctive treatments for treating seizures.”